Introduction to Psychology and education:some essential background
Why do we need psychology?
Virtually
everybody seems to think that they know a lot about psychology, and about how
education
should
be run. After all, most of us have had a lot of experience with other people,
and virtually all of us
have
been to school, or have had some form of experience in which we have learned
from others.
The majority
of our ideas about ‘what works’ are built up from personal experience, and
these beliefs work
well
in our everyday lives. However, they are not necessarily very effective when
they are applied to the
particular
process of educating children. Here, general rules of thumb and common-sense
simplifications can sometimes result in very contradictory perspectives when
applied by different people. It can be
impossible to ‘prove’ which of two
such opposing views is the more valid
What is educational psychology?
Psychological
knowledge and the techniques of psychological study can help us understand
these
problems
since psychology involves the logical investigation of what people think and
what they do.
Psychology
includes a wide range of topics and can be applied to many different areas such
as education.
where
human thinking and behaviour are important. Educational psychology therefore
refers to
an
area of applied psychology that uses psychological theories and techniques to
consider how we
think and learn, and how we can
address the learning needs of students.
Ways of investigating
A
key feature of psychology as a discipline is its emphasis on developing theories
about human behavior and
carrying out investigations to test and modify them. A theory is a way
of trying to explain as
simply
as possible what we know (or think we know) about a particular area. For
example, a theory that
most
people have about class size and achievement is that ‘smaller classes are
better for children and lead to improved achievements’. From this theory we might make
the following prediction (hypothesis):
‘Children
taught in classes no bigger than six will have better end of year test results
than children who
are
taught in classes of 30.’ This process of identifying a theory, making a
prediction that tests it, and
then
collecting data to see if the prediction is supported, is known as the hypothetico-deductive
method, and has its
roots in science. Other techniques, described later in this chapter, are inductive
in
nature:
these approaches actively avoid the initial use of theories, instead allowing
theoretical explanations to
emerge from more open-ended analysis of the data or evidence obtained.
An
experimental investigation of class size could look at what happens when
we change only
the
particular thing (or variable) that we are interested in, in this case
how many children are being
taught
together. For instance, we could investigate the effects of class size on
achievement by setting
up
different-sized groups and measuring children’s progress with their school
work. For us to know
that
class size was the only thing having an effect, we would have to make sure that
all other aspects
of
the classes being compared were as identical as possible, so that we might be
confident that if there
are
differences in achievement, it is because of the difference in class size,
rather than other factors,
such
as who was teaching the class, or the way in which the children were seated in
the class. Such
unintentional
differences between groups in experiments which have the potential to offer an
alternative
explanation
of the results obtained are known as confounding variables.
A
good experimental investigation would set up different-sized classes with matched
groups of
pupils,
to cancel out or ‘control for’ the effects of student ability. Matching is the
process of finding
children
with similar personal characteristics (such as age, gender or general ability),
and allocating them
to
different groups in an experiment, so that the children in each group are
similar to each other. An
alternative
to matching pupils on the basis of ability would be to randomly allocate
students to either a
treatment
group (in which the children receive some form of educational intervention) or
the control
or
comparison groups. A control group is a group used in intervention study
designs that have exactly
the
same experience as the treatment group except for the intervention itself.
Often they receive
‘normal
classroom tuition’ but better studies will give the control group a dummy
intervention to participate
in
(i.e. a ‘treatment’ that we would not expect to have any effect on the
performance variable
being
measured). The reason this is a good idea is that it enables us to control for
any placebo effects.
That
is, students receiving something different from the norm may have an
expectation that this will
benefit
them, and this in itself may motivate them to try harder or affect their
performance in other
ways.
By giving both groups something novel to do, it means that both groups have the
same level of expectation. Another consideration that researchers have to bear
in mind is the potential, indirect,
motivational
effects that extra attention from a researcher may have on student performance.
This is
known
as the Hawthorn Effect. So studies that include a dummy intervention are
also a good idea
because
they enable both sets of children to have similar levels of contact with the
research team –
having
normal classroom tuition as the control experience does not afford this.
There
is a lot to be said for directly setting up different educational experiences
for children, since
the
outcomes may then be assumed to be closely related to what was done. However,
doing so can be
very
difficult in practice. Interfering with children’s education in this way can
also be ethically questionable,
since
children in some of the groups are likely to learn less well. Many educational
investigations
therefore
avoid these problems by using techniques where the investigator uses only
information
that
is already available, or looks at situations that already exist.
Such
non-experimental investigations are typically based on observational
techniques. These
can
involve an investigator directly, perhaps watching children in a class, or be
based on indirect data
such
as school records. Such approaches can sometimes be quasi-experimental (‘quasi’
meaning ‘as
if↜’),
when it is possible to assume that a change in one thing is related to a change
in something else.
‘Natural
experiments’ can make this more likely. For instance, if a new form of
educational practice
(such
as the literacy hour) is introduced, we can compare children’s educational
progress before
and
after its introduction.
One
very common form of observational investigation – perhaps the least
experimental – is to
evaluate
the extent to which one thing naturally varies along with, or correlates with,
something
else.
Such investigations are often easy to carry out and can be fertile ground for
developing new ideas
or
hypotheses about the way things work.
The
main difficulty with such non-experimental approaches is that any outcomes
might not necessarily
be
the result of any change in some other particular measure. For instance, if we
looked only at
existing
classes of different sizes, we could be fooled by the fact that many schools
use small classes for
pupils
of below-average ability. We might then conclude that small classes have the
effect of reducing
attainments!
However,
since such investigations do not involve interference or control by an
investigator, it can
be
argued that they are more likely to be valid, in the sense that they are more
naturalistic, or show
what
normally goes on. They can also lend themselves to personal involvement, and
possibly more
meaningful
interpretation, by an investigator. This happens in participant research,
where the
investigator
might for instance become part of a teaching team. Observational data also fit
well with
the
use of qualitative approaches (see below), with an emphasis on the direct
experiences and interpretations
of those who are involved.
Quantitative and qualitative approaches
A
great deal of educational research involves measuring things. Although such quantitative
approaches
allow us to use powerful statistical techniques, they can often have the effect
of simplifying
and
distorting what is really happening, because things have to be put into
categories of some kind.
Children,
teachers and the processes and outcomes of education are much more than just
sets of numbers.
A
good example is early reading skills, which emphasise decoding using sounds and
letters. These
are
very different from more advanced skills, which involve comprehension and the
use of context. It
could
be very misleading to compare different reading levels along a single scale, as
though higher
attainments were just more of the
same thing.
Qualitative approaches
attempt to get closer to reality by looking at information that differs in
kind
rather than in amount. They may involve using more direct and richer
information, such as the
recording
of complete observations, or descriptions by teachers or pupils about what they
are doing or
how
they feel. This information is close to the way things are, and Glaser and
Strauss (1967) argue
that
it enables researchers to develop a grounded theory, one which arises
from the information
gathered,
rather than just depending on modifying existing theories. However, Silverman
(2005) notes
that
modern qualitative research has two main models or perspectives within it which
are worth being
aware
of. The emotionalist model is primarily interested in looking at a
situation from an individual’s
point
of view, and is interested in perceptions and emotional reactions to
situations. The
counterpoint
to this is the constructionist model, which emphasises what people are
doing, without
necessarily
dwelling on the reasoning or emotions behind those actions.
In
reality, qualitative and quantitative approaches are closely related. Most
quantitative research
involves
qualitative decisions about which variables to study and about what are
appropriate techniques
to
analyse the data. An initial qualitative approach can also develop into a
subsequent quantitative
analysis;
for example, once individuals’ responses have been placed into meaningful
groupings,
these can then be calculated as
percentages or analysed for significant differences.
Describing and analysing findings
With
quantitative data, psychological and educational researchers often use
statistics to describe and
analyse
what they have found. It is useful to have a basic idea of some key statistical
concepts so that
you
can understand and be critical of how the information from investigations has
been interpreted.
The
Appendix to this book explains some of the terms and techniques that are
referred to throughout
the
book.
One
of the greatest errors, but a typical one, is to assume that because the
results of a statistical test
are
‘statistically significant’, this automatically means that the results are
psychologically or educationally
meaningful.
If you understand something about the basic ideas of statistics, you are much
less
likely
to be misled about findings that are marginal or misleading.
Qualitative
information typically takes the form of direct recordings of events and their
meanings,
or
of people’s own descriptions, often referred to as narratives.
Interpreting such diverse information
can
involve selecting key themes and reporting on them by reproducing parts of
transcripts. In one
example,
Walker (1998) carried out an analysis of the functions of secondary school
parents’ evenings,
using
parts of her interviews with parents to demonstrate that such meetings were
almost invariably
perceived
as frustrating and distressing.
Qualitative
analyses often involve setting up possible categories into which the
information can be
placed.
One advantage of having access to the entire range of original information is
that such categories
can
be modified if alternative groupings subsequently appear to be more meaningful.
Although
this
may make conclusions appear rather fluid and unreliable, they can be confirmed
by comparing
the
views found by different types of investigations or information (triangulation),
or by repeating
the
cycle of gathering and analysing information (replication). In any case,
it can be argued that such
approaches
are more likely to result in findings that have some real meaning for a
particular area. As
discussed
later in this chapter (see ‘Shifting paradigms’, pp. 7–10), any categories that
we use can be
seen
as social constructs and are therefore bound to be somewhat arbitrary. However,
qualitative
researchers
value subjective experience and interpretation as valid data which tell us
about how individuals
experience
the world. Whether or not that subjective interpretation is ‘right’ in absolute
terms
is not relevant if there are very
real consequences to how the individual sees the situation they are in
Applying psychology
‘Pure’
psychology tries to arrive at general theories that can help us understand
basic areas such as
learning,
memory, motivation, etc. However, practical education is a complex situation
and there are
often
many factors that interact or combine to give rise to a number of different
effects. For example,
academic
achievement can be the outcome of the interaction between home- and school-based
factors,
with
initial home-based advantages being consolidated by early educational success.
It
is therefore always important to evaluate real-life applications of
psychological ideas, rather than
rely
on ideas that are derived purely from the original abstract theories; these are
often based on work
that
was originally far removed from the realities of real-life teaching. Some of
the early psychological
theories
about learning, for instance, were derived largely from studying the responses
of rats and
pigeons
in mazes and cages!
Differing perspectives
Applying
psychology to education also often involves viewing areas from a number of
different psychological
perspectives
(see Table 1.1). Applying these perspectives to educational topics can generate
alternative
ways of approaching problems. Each of the perspectives generates a very
different way of
understanding
the behaviour of children in school. The various approaches are often
complementary.
For
instance, achieving optimum arousal levels by using a dynamic teaching style
will facilitate general
involvement
with learning tasks. When pupils are more alert, they are then also more likely
to
respond
to other strategies that will focus them on their work, such as the use of
praise in operant
conditioning
(associating a voluntary response with a stimulus).
On
the other hand, some perspectives can give rise to contradictory approaches.
Behaviourism,
for
instance, can appear rather simplistic and may encourage an approach based on
rote learning.
Cognitive
approaches, however, emphasise the use of meaning and understanding, and seem
closer
to
what we personally experience in learning situations. Despite this, behavioural
approaches can
still
be very useful in analysing and managing problem behaviours. Recent developments
consider
that
behavioural conditioning is the result of developing expectancies about what
will happen in
certain
situations, and that behaviourism can therefore be seen as a particular subset
of cognitive
processes.
Developmental psychology
Psychology
also tries to account for the ways in which children establish basic abilities
such as reasoning,
problem-solving
and language use. General developmental theories that cover these can be
applied
to education to help us understand learning situations. This can be seen in
Chapter 2, which
considers
the role of Piaget’s theory of cognitive development, and Chapter 9, which
looks at the way
in
which language abilities are developed. Other areas, such as the development of
social roles and
identity,
and the establishment of basic academic attainments such as reading, also
depend to some
extent on progress with other
underlying skills and abilities.
The importance of theory
There
is a famous remark by Allport (1947) that the aims of science are ‘understanding,
prediction
and€control,
above the levels achieved by unaided common sense’. This perspective is very
useful in guiding psychological investigations, and emphasises that we should
be able to use theoretical knowledge
to
help us with applied areas and to go beyond everyday experience and
understanding.
Developments
in education often lack this theoretical foundation and are frequently inspired
by
social
processes or ideological beliefs, a fact that can lead to cycles of change as
the general social climate
alters.
For instance, in the 1940s it was commonly believed that the most efficient way
of educating
children
was to select them for different types of schooling using the ‘eleven-plus’ and
also to
‘stream’
them into different general ability groups. A later ideological emphasis on
equality of opportunity
subsequently
led to the development of comprehensive schools and mixed-ability teaching.
However,
there are now signs that there is a shift backwards in this perspective, with
many schools
reverting
to increased selection and ability grouping of pupils, even at the primary
level.
A
psychological perspective could help us to limit such swings of fashion by
providing theories and
knowledge
about the realistic advantages and disadvantages of such developments. For
instance, it has
been
shown that selection of pupils on the basis of the eleven-plus (an intelligence
test) is not a very
accurate
or useful process. Research also indicates that streaming of children into
different ability
groups
within schools leads to only limited improvements with the higher groups. It
can also lead to
pupils
in lower groups receiving inferior education, partly because of teacher
expectations, and the
negative social groupings that can
happen in such classes.
Further reading
Breakwell, Hammond, Fife-�Shaw and Smith (eds) (2006), Research Methods in Psychology: a
more
detailed text covering both qualitative and quantitative approaches to
psychological research.
A
helpful book if you are already familiar with research methods, but need
specific guidance.
Greig, Taylor and MacKay (2007), Doing Research with Children: a good basic
introduction to
designing
research projects that involve children as participants.
Martin, Carlson and Buskist (2010), Psychology: an introductory text for those
who are unfamiliar
with psychology as a general
discipline.
Komentar
Posting Komentar